Published
in: M. Gelfand, C. Chiu, Y. Hong (Eds.), Advances in Culture and Psychology (vol. 3, chap. 6).
N.Y.: Oxford University Press, 2012.
Macro
Cultural Psychology: Its Development, Concerns, Politics, and Future Direction
Carl Ratner
http://www.sonic.net/~cr2
Abstract
This
chapter explains how psychological phenomena originate in macro cultural
factors, embody their characteristics, and function to promulgate them. A
variety of psychological phenomena are used to illustrate this theory:
identity, sexuality, educational psychology, sense of
time, emotions, agency, racism, numeracy, gender, and autobiographical memory.
The chapter emphasizes political aspects of cultural factors, psychological phenomena,
and the discipline of psychology. An epistemology and a qualitative methodology
are outlined for elucidating cultural origins, characteristics, and functions
of psychology.
Keywords:
qualitative methodology, gender, politics, sexuality, educational psychology,
memory, agency, racism, objectivism
I. Introduction
Macro
cultural psychology elucidates the dialectical relation between psychological
phenomena and macro cultural factors such as social institutions, cultural artifacts,
and cultural concepts. Macro cultural psychology aims to develop this relationship
into a complete, comprehensive paradigm for explaining, describing, and
predicting human psychology. It does not simply empirically establish cultural
influences on psychology. Rather it establishes that psychological phenomena
are essentially cultural. Culture is the essential element of psychology,
the basis of psychology, psychologyÕs reason for
existence. Macro cultural factors are the locus where psychology is formed; macro
cultural factors comprise psychologyÕs leading characteristics, mechanisms,
dynamics, and function. Psychological phenomena embody and perform macro
cultural factors. Macro culture is the Ògrand narrativeÓ of human psychology
just as natural selection is the Ògrand narrativeÓ of biology. Human psychology
does not originate in interpersonal processes, infantile processes, animal
processes, or innate, Óhard-wiredÓ biological processes. This chapter explores these
tenets of this integral psychological theory.
In
addition to being a psychological theory, macro cultural psychology is a
cultural theory about what culture is, what its main factors are, what its
dynamics are, what its organization is, and why it is important for human
development -- i.e., why/how culture civilizes human faculties to make
them distinctive, higher conscious functions.
I
emphasize that culture consists fundamentally of macro cultural factors –
social institutions, cultural artifacts, and cultural concepts. They have
concrete, variable content in particular societies. When we say that human
psychological phenomena are cultural, we mean that they are organized by macro
cultural factors. ). I will cite evidence that the dominant macro cultural
factor is the political economy. This makes all macro cultural factors and
their corresponding psychology political to some degree.
Cultural factors are internally related
to each other: they are interdependent and permeate each otherÕs
character/quality. Cultural factors form a coherent cultural system. Any
particular cultural and psychological factor, such as education, is
ÒoverdeterminedÓ by numerous other elements of the system. Any factor/element
is Òa total social phenomenon...at once legal, economic, religious, esthetic,
morphological, and so onÓ (Mauss, 1967, p. 76).
This
chapter will outline the historical ancestry of macro cultural psychology
(section II), the organic interdependence of macro cultural factors and
psychology (section III), how psychological phenomena are barometers of culture
and reflect their positive and negative features (section lV), that culture is
political which shapes the content of psychology and the socialization of psychology
(section V), that society needs psychology to maintain itself (section VI), that
psychological phenomena perform cultural maintenance (section VII), an
epistemology for macro cultural psychology, (section VIII), and qualitative
methodology for macro cultural psychology (section VIII).
II.
Historical Background of Macro Cultural Psychology and Key Assumptions
The basic idea of macro cultural psychology was articulated
by Marx in his statement that
a specific mode of production of objects entails a specific form of
subjectivity: Òproduction not only creates an object for the subject, but also
a subject for the object. Production thus produces not only the object but also
the manner of consumption, not only objectively but also subjectively.
Production not only supplies a material for the need, but it also supplies a
need for the material. It thus produces the object of consumption, the manner
of consumption, and the motive of consumption.Ó Marx (1973, pp. 494, 495) when he said, "Not only do the
objective conditions change in the act of reproduction, e.g., the village
becomes a town, the wilderness a cleared field, etc., but the producers change
too in that they bring out new qualities in themselves, develop themselves in
production, transform themselves, develop new powers and ideas, new modes of
intercourse, new needs, and new language" (Marx, 1973, pp. 92, 494, 495). Subjectivity is part of objective
cultural factors; it is formed by them and in them, it
has their character, and it functions to support them.
Vygotsky
espoused this concept in a paper written in 1929, entitled ÒConcrete
PsychologyÓ -- a term he took from the French Marxist philosopher-psychologist
Georges Politzer (see also Kosik, 1976 for an important treatise on concrete
psychology and philosophy). Vygotsky said, ÒWe derive individual functions from
forms of collective life. Development proceeds not toward socialization, but
toward individualization of social functions (transformation of social
functions into psychological functions)Ó (Vygotsky, 1989, p. 61).
A. N.
Leontiev (1977) further explained the idea that psychology is a macro cultural
phenomenon:
Despite
all its diversity, all its special features, the activity [Tatigkeit] of
the human individual is a system that obeys the system
of relations of society. Outside these relations human activity does not exist.
How it exists is determined by the forms and means of
material and spiritual communication that are generated by the development of
production and that cannot be realised except in the activity of specific
individuals. It stands to reason that the activity of every individual depends
on his place in society, on his conditions of life.Ó ÒAlthough a scientific
psychology must never lose sight of man's inner world, the study of this inner
world cannot be divorced from a study of his activity and does not constitute
any special trend of scientific psychological investigation (see Ratner, 2012a,
b, d,; Ratner & Guitart, 2011, for additional
roots of macro cultural psychology).
Macro
cultural psychology develops this perspective into a specific, coherent,
comprehensive cultural-psychological theory. Macro cultural psychology thus
operates within the framework of grand, classical theories.
The
American Psychological AssociationÕs Task Force on Socioeconomic Status (2002,
pp. 1, 25, 26) is an excellent illustration of these macro cultural
psychological principles. It emphasizes that the political economy of a society
– i.e., socioeconomic status, or class -- is a major constituent of human
psychology.
The
Task Force report states that socioeconomic factors and social class are
fundamental determinants of human functioning across the life span, including
development, well-being, and physical and mental
health. These should be primary concerns for psychological research, practice,
education, policy, and advocacy.
For
example, the increasing stratification of American society since the 1970s has
exacerbated psychological differences among rich and poor: (a) the gap between
rich and poor children on standardized test scores is now 40 percent bigger
than it was in 1970. That is double the testing gap between black and white
children. Class is thus a more powerful social and psychological differentiator
than race; (b) the gap between rich and poor in college completion — one
of the single most important predictors of economic success — has grown
by more than 50 percent since the 1990s. More than half of children from
high-income families finish college, up from about a third 20 years ago. Fewer
than 10 percent of low-income children finish, up from 5 percent (Tavernise,
2011, 2012; Ratner, 2012a, pp. 207-210).
Psychology
must avoid contributing to the injuries of social class. This is a scientific
issue for research and treatment/intervention, as well as a political
recommendation for social reform: ÒWe must broaden psychologyÕs advocacy agenda
beyond the promotion of programs and policies that exclusively focus on
psychological health to include those that direct access to material resources,
while remaining vigilant to the social psychological and cultural meaning of
these policies and programs...We challenge psychology as a field to think
broadly about how our work does or does not advance broad-based structural
change.Ó ÒInterventions [with poor individuals] can significantly improve the
well-being of those whose lives are touched by [difficulties]; at the same time,
they do little to address classism or the fundamental causes of inequality at
the social level, which will ultimately limit the effectiveness of such
interventions in reaching all those in needÓ (APA, 2002, p. 25).
The
remainder of this chapter will explain just how macro cultural factors are
political, how they organize psychology, and how psychology is a
culture-performing competency. We begin by explaining how macro cultural
factors and psychology are organically related – a unity of differences, or
a differentiated unity.
III.
Macro Cultural Factors and Psychology Are Organically Related
The
relation of macro cultural factors and psychology is not an inter-action of
independent phenomena (realms). The relation is an internal, integral,
dialectical one of interdependent phenomena. Psychology is part of macro
cultural factors and macro cultural factors are part of psychology. They are
two sides of the same coin. Psychology is the subjectivity of macro
cultural factors, and macro cultural factors are the objectivity of
psychology -- i.e., the objectification of psychology in artifacts, cultural
concepts, and social institutions. Macro cultural factors
contain living subjectivity, they are not reified
things. Conversely, subjectivity/consciousness/psychology embodies objective
cultural factors; they are not ethereal, immaterial, subjectivity.
Psychology
is cultural because human life is cultural. Culture is the basis of our
humanity and civilization, it is our survival
mechanism and fulfillment mechanism. Our behavior/psychology is directed toward
constructing culture which is the essence of our
humanity. We are culture-makers, not simply meaning-makers. The meaning of
human life is to construct culture. Culture is meaningful,
meaning is not a personal construct apart from culture. And the cornerstones of
culture are macro cultural factors. Meaning is primarily made through producing
macro cultural factors. These are the meanings that filter down to
interpersonal relations. Culture requires – selects for – a unique
kind of psychology/behavior. Animal, or infantile, instincts cannot produce
culture. Human psychology is the behavioral mechanism that makes macro culture
and is selected and formed by macro culture. Psychology is therefore a cultural
phenomenon.
Macro
cultural factors are the locus where psychological phenomena are
crafted, administered, objectified, and socialized/transmitted. In addition,
the need to construct/maintain macro cultural factors -- schools, economy,
government, art, science -- is the impetus, telos,
and raison dÕetre of psychological development. In these ways psychology
takes the form of macro cultural factors.
A. MACRO CULTURAL FACTORS ARE THE
BASIS, CHARACTER, OPERATING MECHANISM, AND DRIVING FORCE OF PSYCHOLOGY/BEHAVIOR
1. The
orgasm as macro cultural psychological phenomenon
The
most intimate and personal experiences are structured and permeated by macro
cultural factors, and function to promulgate them. This is what civilizes
personal, intimate experience, in contrast to simple, automatic, stereotyped bio-physical reactions that characterizes animal and
infantile behavior. The cultural essence of intimate personal experience is
manifested in the sexual orgasm.
Fahs
(2011, pp. 9, 53, 57-61) interviewed 40 American women about sex and reported that Òwhen speaking with women about their
sexualities, women disclosed all sorts of [social] performances, many of which reinscribed
rather than unsettled traditional gender roles. Women discussed performing
[sex] as a means to make their partners happy, to satisfy ideas of what women should
do or should be, to mesh with cultural expectations that they felt they
could not meet.Ó ÒWhen talking with women directly about orgasm, a prominent
theme of feeling pressure to orgasm emerged, as women struggled both to feel
pleasure and to demonstrate their pleasure to sexual partners.Ó 25-year old
Kate said, ÒPutting pressure on myself to orgasm feels strange. For something
thatÕs supposed to happen spontaneously, it feels like thereÕs a lot of thought
put into this and a lot of anxiety around it, like this is some kind of
benchmark of not only the sex, but who this person it as a lover, or how I am
as a lover, or how I am as a woman.Ó A 46-year old said, ÒDuring times where I
have not been able to orgasm, I felt worthless and angry at myself...Ó A
25-year old said, ÒI feel pressure from myself to orgasm because I just feel
like thereÕs all that energy, like work thatÕs being done that doesnÕt
go anywhere...He wants to know that he can bring this about for me, and I want
to show him that.Ó ÒSince [my orgasm] is such a big factor in how guys value
themselves, if I care about the person, IÕll [fake] it...IÕm not going to fake
it if I donÕt really care.Ó
These
comments reveal that orgasms are cultural acts, fraught with cultural meanings,
ideals, pressures, objectives, identity, emotions, benchmarks
of normal functioning, social appearances, and striving. Women use orgasms to
live up to cultural ideals, to validate their partners and themselves; to express
care about their partners; to gain a productive return on their investment in
sexual work (so all that work isnÕt wasted); they feel angry and worthless when
they do not achieve culturally-valued orgasms; they put pressure on themselves,
they try to achieve orgasms, and many women fake orgasms for
cultural-psychological reasons (to make their partners orgasm, to end the
sexual encounter, to make their lover feel virile). Orgasms are cultural
performances of cultural standards; they involve conscious psychological
processes, thinking, effort, self-consciousness, self-praise and self-criticism
(Camoletto,
2011)..
WomenÕs
sexual phantasies are also fraught with cultural content (ibid., p. 246ff.): Group sex and threesomes; romance; inaccessible
people with status (such as celebrities, men in uniform, married men, strangers);
sex in taboo, risky places; dominating others; submitting to others -- e.g., rape,
prostitution, sexual objectification. Submission was the most common sexual
fantasy, reported by 1/3 to 1/2 of women in various surveys. Even women who
have been abused and raped have these kinds of erotic fantasies. (Cultural stereotypes
of womenÕs subordination to men overpower negative personal experiences with
this gender relation.)
Cultural ideals, pressures, agency, validation, concerns,
and social relations are what lead women to have, not have, or pretend to have
an orgasm -- only about one-half of American women regularly experience orgasms
during intercourse. They are the operating mechanisms of the organism. The
orgasm is a Òtechnology of the self,Ó which Foucault defined as a cultural
means for constructing the self (Ratner, 2012a, pp. 159-160).
Sex is a social process, it is
not a mechanical physical, natural act. Lovers discuss what excites them, to
make it more enjoyable. They read sex manuals and get sexual counseling. If sex
were natural none of this would occur.
Sex is additionally a culturally-organized
psychological phenomena that animates cultural behavior and achieves cultural
goals. Culturally-organized sex promulgates culturally
valued social relationships with partners possessing culturally ideal features
(behavioral, anatomical, social, financial/material). Our sexual partners
personify socially valued physical features, material status, demeanors, and
sexual behaviors.[1]
2. Biology, culture, and psychology
With sexual desire and sexual responsiveness being
culturally organized, biology must play a subordinate role in human sexuality,
and in all human psychology/behavior. This is a central principle of macro
cultural psychological theory. Biology must lose the determining role it plays
in animal and infant behavior in order that culture can organize
behavior/psychology. Innate biological mechanisms determine mechanical,
automatic, stereotyped behavior. This prevents cultural, conscious formation of
behavior/psychology. The co-existence of discrepant, competing processes is
unworkable and violates the law of parsimony. Biology cannot operate at the
same level in the same determining manner as culture vis
a vis psychology. Culture displaces biology to the general level of energizing psychology/behavior,
while culture takes over determining the specific character or quality of psychology/behavior.
There is a hiatus between biology and culture in that biological
processes energize but do not determine or explain psychology/behavior (Ratner,
2012, pp. 107-129). The mechanisms of behavior/psychology are cultural, as in
the case of sex.
Ratner (1991, pp.199-242) reported several important
facts about the subordination of biological processes to cultural ones in human
psychology: Low socioeconomic status men with low to normal levels of
testosterone are more likely to engage in antisocial behavior than are upper
class men with high levels of testosterone. Only 4% of upper class, high
testosterone men are delinquent as adults compared with 15% of lower class men
with normal testosterone levels. 14% percent of lower class men with normal
testosterone levels were delinquents in childhood, in comparison with 11% of
high testosterone, upper class men.
Regarding sex, normal prepubescent animals (males and
females) whose gonadal hormones have not begun secreting show
no sexual activity (except in the case of apes where pre-pubertal males
and females do often copulate). Human children (especially in societies that
encourage them) indulge in sexual activities years before gonadal hormones have
begun functioning. Girls completely lacking in any kind of ovarian hormone
describe daydreams and fantasies of romantic courtship, marriage, and
autoerotic genital play.
Removal of animals' gonadal hormones through castration
or ovariectomy prevents adult sexual activity if done before puberty, or eliminates
it if done after puberty. Reduction in hormonal levels has little if any affect
on human sexual behavior. Ovariectomy and menopause in a high proportion of
women produce no change in sexual desire, just as oral contraceptives, which inhibit
ovarian, hypothalamic, and pituitary hormones, have no inhibiting affect on sexual
activity (and increase it!).
Healthy males show a wide range of testosterone values
(from about 350 to 1000 nanograms per 100 milliliters of blood) and variations
within this range have no significance for sexual behavior. Castration of males
sometimes leads to reduced interest in sex, however many individuals maintain
an undiminished sexual drive and coital ability for several decades.
Cortical processes and culture. Macro
cultural psychology argues that the cortex is organized by culture, not vice versa.
This was VygotskyÕs position. If culture is to be the mechanism of psychology,
it must organize the indeterminate cortical processing of psychology. The
cultural features of psychology determine how and where it is
processed by the cortex. The cortex cannot consist of Òhard-wired,Ó
pre-determined modules for processing psychology, because psychology does not
have a pre-determined form that could be ÒknownÓ or programmed by the cortex in
advance of its cultural form. (Hard-wired, pre-programmed, innate determination
of behavior only makes sense when the behavior is fundamentally fixed, as in
animal behavior that confronts a regular, slow-changing natural environment.
Culture is rapidly changing through social construction-innovation, and it
requires variable behavior/psychology that cannot be innately programmed or
processed.) Tang, et al. (2006) measured the cortical localization of simple
addition tasks using Arabic numbers among native English speakers (NES) and
Chinese speakers (NCS) (Ss averaged 24-27 years old). Cultural localization was
substantially different in both the right hemisphere and the left hemisphere,
although some overlap in localization among the two
cultural groups also existed. In NES, the visuo-premotor pathway was extended to the Broca
area, implicating strong verbal dependence of the math fact retrieval that may
be mediated by phonological processing. In NCS, however, the retrieval
processing showed a much weaker activation in the Broca area, suggesting that
the area for verbal processing was not engaged. This data proves that there is
not an innate, universal cortical location (module) that processes arithmetic.
Instead, the cortex is organized to process arithmetic in different loci by
cultural processes that differ in different societies.
3. The error of eclecticism-interactionism
Attempts to eclectically combine innate biological
determinants of psychology with culture are futile because they fail to
appreciate fundamental differences between the two.
A case in point is Matsumoto & HwangÕs (2012) attempt
at a biocultural theory of emotions. The authors posit
Òthe existence of a biologically innate processing system that activates some
emotional statesÉThis core emotion system is hard-wired, fairly impermeable to
modification by experience, and relatively unchanged throughout the lifespanÓ
(p. 97). Universal facial expressions and physiological responses associated
with particular emotions are said to support this claim. The authors also claim
an animal basis of natural, universal emotions. In addition, the authors posit
cultural emotions. We cannot examine all these details here (see Ratner, 1989,
2000, 2007a for a discussion of naturalistic emotions). We simply observe that
the authors present cultural data that contradicts their claim for biological
emotions and supports the macro cultural psychological theory of culture,
biology, and psychology.
The authors state that cultures influence biological
emotions (p. 98): Òthere is likely no emotion that is entirely biologically
driven without cultural influenceÓ (p. 108). One reason is that unmodified
biological Òemotions such as anger and disgust are potentially destructive
emotions in any culture. For these reasons, people of all cultures minimize the
expression of these emotions toward higher status others or to ingroup membersÓ
(p. 99). One way of minimizing biological emotions is to manipulate display
rules. Another way to modify emotions is by manipulating the interpretation of
a situation so that different emotions may be stimulated by it.
All this cultural modification of ÒcoreÓ Òbiological
emotionsÓ negates the claim that they are hard-wired, fairly impermeable to
modification by experience, and relatively unchanged throughout the lifespan.
The cultural manipulations of display rules clearly contradict the claim of
Òmorphological similarities between human and nonhuman primate expressionsÓ (p.
97).
The authorsÕ admission that humans can feign
ÒcoreÓ emotions when they are not elicited by a natural stimulus additionally
refutes their contention that core emotions are natural, automatic, mechanical
responses.
Anthropological evidence reveals that the
quality/experience of anger, sadness, and guilt are culturally organized and
variable. Buddhist anger, sadness, and guilt are qualitatively different from
Western forms of these Òcore, biologicalÓ emotions (Ratner, 1991, pp. 76-83; Ratner,
2006, pp. 106-108; Ratner, 1997, pp. 105-106; Ratner, 2007a; Ratner 2012a;
Stearns & Stearns, 1985; Stearns, 2011, 2006, 2005, 2003, 1999, 1994, 1990). If
Òcore biological emotionsÓ can be and need be culturally modified and imbued
with cultural content and capacities, what is left of their natural, immutable,
hard-wired biological core?
This discussion demonstrates that culture is not
compatible with natural emotions that are hard-wired according to pre-cultural
conditions. Culture displaces natural determinants of behavior with
qualitatively different mechanisms, properties, and capacities – such as
responding to abstract symbolic meanings about distant events never encountered.
Macro cultural psychology is not claiming that
culture eliminates biology from emotions or psychology. Obviously, the
brain and hormones are necessary. However, culture transforms biologyÕs role in
behavior to a general potentiating substratum that does not determine specific
behavior/psychology the way that it does in animals and infants. Culture
neither eliminates, nor adds to, nor interacts with natural processes; it
transforms them. This conclusion is the heart of VygotskyÕs sociocultural
psychology (Ratner, 1991, 1998, 2004, 2012a).
IV. Psychological Phenomena Are Barometers
of Culture
Psychology
is an indicator, or barometer of the macro cultural factors that form it.
Consider parentsÕ feelings toward their children that we discussed in footnote
#1. We said that parental feelings reflect and reinforce the cultural value of
educational success. However, for some groups of parents, their feelings toward
children are independent of the childrenÕs educational success. Macro cultural
psychology would use the parentsÕ subjective, personal feelings as a barometer
of the social structure, as an indication that the social structure does not
value the educational success of these classes of children.
For
instance, the American economy for the past 4 decades has made occupational
success very difficult for the working class; consequently there is no economic
need for working class children to succeed in school, and there is no cultural-psychological
need for their parents to motivate them to succeed by including success as a
component of their pride in children. Indeed, encouraging working class children
to succeed in school (through building this into parental emotions) would
create false hopes and lead to resentment and rebellion at the lack of job
opportunities for using school success. It is socially functional for lower
class parents to exclude school success from their parenting in a hierarchical,
class society that devalues them and their children. Society, not parents, should
be blamed for parentsÕ emotions.
V. Culture Is Political, and Cultural
Politics Shape The Content of Psychology and The Socialization of Psychology
Culture
is political. Therefore, power/politics must be a central category of cultural
psychology. Long before Foucault, Bertrand Russell
(1938, p. 10) described the fundamental concept in the social science as power
– in the same sense in which energy is the fundamental concept in
physics.
The
sociological field of institutionalism emphasizes power/politics at the heart
of cultural factors and the subjectivity they promote (Friedland, 2009, p. 38).
Power/politics not only produces macro cultural factors, it also produces
subjectivity. This is known as subjectification. Subjectification
includes ÒsubjectivationÓ: how individuals form their subjectivity on the basis
of various forms of administration (Foucault, 1997; Barry, et al., 1996;
Burchell, et al., 1991; Mansfield, 2007)).
A.
TIME
A
detailed example of the way political macro cultural factors organize
psychology is the way the psychology of time has been structured by political
economic interests. Historian Giordano Nanni (2011, 2012) finds evidence in the
colonization of Aboriginal people by the British in Victoria, Australia. The
territorial conquest was accompanied by colonizing the Aboriginal sense of
time; the dispossession of material resources entailed the dispossession of
psychology.
Nanni
begins with a bold statement that, Òfrom 1492, the histories of western time
and western imperialism are virtually inseparableÓ (2011, p. 6). Subsequent
19th-century
European settler-colonial expansion projects to displace and reform
ÔalternativeÕ temporalities outside Europe were Þrst overtly deployed as a
means of establishing control. There is no doubt that the most patent
manifestation of the intimate connection of time with Empire lies in the official
deployment of Greenwich Mean Time in 1884 – that grandiloquent gesture of
temporal imperialism, par excellence – which at the height of the
colonial era sought to replace the miscellany of Ôlocal timesÕ around the world
with a single, centralized notion of Ôstandard timeÕ...
But
on a cultural level, the trend towards temporal centralization and
standardization was paralleled by an attack on local, Indigenous temporal
systems, whose perceived ÔirregularityÕ threatened the hegemony of dominant
colonial notions of ÔorderÕ with alternative and conßicting attitudes to time,
work and productivity. During everyday colonial encounters – and
particularly during the course of the evangelization, education and employment
of colonized peoples – colonial societies vigorously pursued the
elimination and/or reform of alternative and incompatible temporal structures. The
ability to impose the observance of speciÞc timetables, rituals and routines
embodied a highly signiÞcant aspect of Empire. The project did not go
uncontested on the ground, however, as colonized people often managed either to
defy the imposition of clock-governed routines and Christian rituals, to
negotiate compromises between the new and old rhythms, or to exploit the
temporal discourses of their self-styled reformers. (ibid.,
pp. 6-7).
The
British did not simply allow the nativesÕ psychology to follow changes in
activity. They actively engineered psychology in order to augment the changes
in activity. They forced on the natives the linear sense of time divided into
equal temporal units on the clock that was appropriate to Western cultural
activity and institutions. Quantified, measurable time was important as the
measurable basis of economic value on the market (the labor theory of value).
In settlements where capitalist economic relations were not central, the
natives were left to maintain their indigenous time to a greater extent (ibid., p. 12).
Time
was political in that it incarnated cultural features that promoted cultural
behavior of the British social order.
The
transformation of time sense was reinforced by tying it to a new sense of
character that was defined by punctual, consistent work, and was compromised by
idleness and indolence. Time sense was integrated into the psychological system
and this, as well as financial rewards, anchored the new sense of time.
Factory
owners in England and other countries applied similar techniques to impose a
capitalist psychology of time upon English factory workers who had emigrated
from rural culture. Contemporary schools also impose time rules on pupils,
praise obedience as a sign of good character, and punish pupils for disobeying.
Socializing a cultural form of time psychology,
socialized the individual into the cultural system of which it is a functional
element.
After a
psychology of time has been socialized, people become habituated to it and
internalize it as their own desire and sense of life. The capitalist time sense
leads people to become impatient to do things quickly, even their own things,
like driving, conducting banking transaction, paying for groceries, and
communicating. Time sense becomes integrated in our psychological system of
motivation, perception, emotions, memory, and self-concept. Our psychological
system then works to implement the cultural sense of time throughout our
cultural activities.
Because
psychology is political, people can resist particular politics by adopting counter-cultural
forms of psychology. This requires knowing what kinds of psychology concretely
negate unwanted politics. Not any and all psychological constructions
effectively resist cultural norms.
The
Australian Aboriginals refused to obey time requirements as a way of retaining
their Aboriginal culture. Their resistance was crushed by the
colonizersÕ superior resources. However, colonial control over time was
never total.
B.
EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
Educational
psychology is another complex of cognitive processes, perception, identity,
emotionality, motivation, and attention/concentration that is organized by
macro cultural factors.
Earlier
we saw that economic inequality has increased the gap between rich and poor
children on standardized test scores 40 percent over what it was in 1970.
Consumerism
and mass media promulgate psychological processes in young people that affect
their educational psychology. Consumerism and mass media foster superficial
tastes and thinking, impulsiveness, immediacy, and sensationalism. This
subjectivity contradicts the subjectivity necessary to concentrate on profound,
complex, and serious academic material (Arum & Roksa, 2011).
A
societyÕs occupational possibilities also determine the kind of educational
psychology that students strive to develop for succeeding in society after
graduation.
StudentsÕ
educational psychology is also affected by multiple features of the educational
system that actually emanate from the political-economy.
To understand the educational institution and its impact on educational
psychology, we must comprehend its political-economic origins, characteristics
and function.
Foster
(2011) documents how recent educational changes in the U.S. reflect and
reinforce the political-economy of late capitalism.
These changes revolve around privatizing education to make it into a cash cow
for corporate profit. This requires commodifying all aspects of education so
that they may be bought and sold to generate profit. Commodification
encompasses constructing and maintaining school buildings, administration,
teacher training, tutoring services, tests, and online curricula. (Already nine
companies account for about 90% of the American educational testing market.)
Commodification
includes transforming the labor of teachers into a free market commodity that
can be rented and disposed of at will by management. TeachersÕ labor is
transformed from one that allows substantial autonomy and personal interaction
with students, into one that treats teachers as semi-skilled, low wage
employees, performing standardized work that is subject to autocratic
management.
Neoliberal
restructuring of education has an additional economic objective -- to prepare
students for low-skill, low-wage, low-autonomy, low-creativity, unintellectual
occupations, controlled by capitalists. It is no coincidence that the
corporations, foundations, and think tanks – such as the American
Legislative Exchange Council and the (Wal-Mart) Walton Foundation -- who
support privatized education, also attack labor rights among workers –and
support religion (see Ratner, 2012a, b; 2013f).
Neoliberal
education includes an ideological component to indoctrinate teachers and
students into capitalist thinking and behavior, including the acceptance of
capitalist commercialism throughout all areas of social life. This requires new
pedagogy that will be described momentarily.
Neoliberal education is perpetrated by political processes. It is
designed and implemented by capitalist corporations, their foundations, and
their political minions in government (New York Times, Aug. 1, 2010,
ÒThe Academic-Industrial ComplexÓ). This educational direction is neither the
will of the people, nor is it blind market forces working themselves out. For
example, The Global Agreement on Trade in Services mandates that all aspects of
education are subject to global free trade and cannot be protected by national
or local governments.
The
political-economic basis, locus, character, and function of neoliberal
educational reform is evident in the following details: In 2005, a group of
billionaire equity investors and hedge fund managers formed Democrats for
Educational Reform to promote ideas such as school choice through charter
schools. Their strategy was to leverage their investments by funding key
Democrats who would share their agenda. One of these was a new Senator from
Chicago, Barack Obama. He helped launch the groupÕs opening event on June 3,
2005. Immediately after he was elected President, the group wrote him a memo
naming its choice for Secretary of Education: Arnie Duncan. Obama dutifully
appointed him, along with Larry Summers (who had pushed for banking deregulation
as Treasury Secretary under Clinton) and neoliberal economists as his economic
team (Ravitch, 2011).
Duncan
was the perfect man for their strategy because he had been ChicagoÕs school
chief and had headed ChicagoÕs Renaissance 2010 that was financed with $90
million from the Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation. The Gates Foundation has
over $35 billion in assets which it uses to promote neoliberal education
including charter schools, breaking teacher unions, privatization, and rote
testing.
Until
2009, Duncan and Summers were on the board of
directors of the reactionary Broad Foundation, which specializes in training
educators to promote neoliberal educational reform. (The Broad Foundation
receives millions from the Gates Foundation.) It was started
by Eli Broad, net worth $5.4 billion from real estate and insurance. In
2009, the Broad Foundation had trained 43% of all large urban school
superintendents. (Many of the superintendents whom Broad trains are ex- senior
military officials and corporate CEOs.) In 2002, Duncan, as CEO of Chicago
school system, worked with the Broad Foundation and other venture capitalists
to initiate a corporate-friendly training program of future school
superintendents. It was the prototype of the Broad FoundationÕs Superintendents
Academy. The Gates Foundation funneled $63.2 million into the Chicago schools
during Duncan's tenure as Chicago schools CEO from 2001-2008.
Duncan
packed the Dept. of Education with Gates and Broad associates. He tapped top
Gates Foundation officers to be his chief of staff and to head the agency's
Office of Innovation and Improvement. Foundation officers are also spearheading
the $4.35 billion Race to the Top program. Duncan also used a Gates-funded
report on education, entitled Turnaround Challenge, as the basis of
ObamaÕs ÒRace To The TopÓ educational reform for public schools. ÒRace To The
TopÓ offered major funding to schools that agreed to restrict teacher tenure,
to rote testing, privatizing, and transforming public schools into charter
schools -- which are publicly funded but are managed by unelected directors who
are free from government regulations (Foster, 2011, p. 24).
The single
biggest investor in charter schools in the United States is the Walton Family
Foundation of Wal-Mart which spent a total of $150.3
million during 2007-08. In New York, the Walton group has provided $15 million
in construction funding plus more than $1 million per year for operating costs
in recent years to help the Brighter Choice charter school network establish
eight new schools in Albany. Meanwhile, Democratic Governor David Paterson has
received contributions totaling $55,900 from Christy Walton, as he pushes
legislation to lift New York's current statewide cap of 200 charter schools.
Larry Ellison, CEO of Oracle, net worth $27 billion, has also funded companies
to promote charter schools.
Corporations
recognize, far more acutely than social scientists, that society is a network
of interconnected macro cultural factors. They astutely promote their interests
in educational reform by supporting related social activities. For example, in
2009, Bill Gates wanted to help New York City mayor Bloomberg to gain a third
term because, among other commonalities, Bloomberg supported GatesÕ neoliberal
educational agenda. (One of the companies that manages
charter schools in Harlem, N.Y., named Success Charter Network, has a board of
9 members, 7 of whom are hedge fund managers. There are no parents, teachers,
or community members on this board.) Therefore, Gates spent $4 million to fund
a political change in New York CityÕs statute that had limited mayoral terms to
two.
Neoliberal
efforts have proven remarkably effective in turning education into a reproducer
of the social class hierarchy. The entering freshmen of the class of 2010 at
the countryÕs 193 most selective colleges contained only 5% from the poorest
25% of the population, 15 percent came from the bottom half of the income
distribution, and sixty-seven percent came from the highest-earning fourth of
the distribution. Only 44 percent of low-income high school seniors with high standardized test scores enroll in a four-year college,
compared with about 50 percent of high-income seniors who have average test
scores (Leonhardt, 2011).
As
government subsidies decline, students are thrown ever more onto the private
resources of their families. Wealthier families always triumph in this
situation. Budget shortfalls have prompted Medina,
Ohio Senior High to impose fees on students who enroll in many academic classes
and extracurricular activities. Parents pay to register their children for
basic courses such as Spanish I and Earth Sciences, to get them into graded
electives such as band, and to allow them to run cross-country and track. A
family's total tab for a year of public education: $4,446.50. At Marietta High
in southeastern Ohio, it costs $33 to take chemistry, $36 for honors chemistry
and $152 for the Advanced Placement course. Dakota Ridge High in Littleton,
Colo., charges sophomores $15 for basic 10th grade
English but $50 for honors. Juniors can take basic
English for $8 or pay $75—plus a test fee of about $90—for Advanced
Placement English Literature (Simon, 2011).
These
public school financial restrictions on educational access feed the class
structure of colleges that favors wealthier students who have been able to
afford to enroll in college prep courses.
Privatization,
like choice and the market, is code for class warfare and class structure. This
is expressed in the etymology of privatization: it comes from the Latin word
Òprivare,Ó which means Òto deprive!Ó
Privatization, choice, and the market have resulted in
socioeconomic status becoming more important as a determinant of educational
success. The percentage of age-15 math and reading achievement test scores
explained by SES is twice as great in the United States and the United Kingdom
as in Canada or Finland, and three times as great as in Japan and Korea where
public support for education is high (Ratner, 2012a, pp. 208-209). Thus, SES is
not a fixed variable, it is politically variable (a
political variable).
1.
Neoliberal pedagogy
The neoliberal approach to education retards educational
advancement. Data proves that charter schools do not uplift low motivation or
inferior educational competencies; nor do charter schools equalize the
competencies of disadvantaged students with well-to-do students. Philadelphia
recently declared that its charter schools were a failure. To earn profit,
charter schools underpay teachers $15,000 a year compared with public school
teachers. The teacher attrition rate in charter schools is 25%, about double
the rate for public schools. Student attrition rate is also higher in charter
schools than public schools (Foster, 2011, pp. 24-27).
Stanford
UniversityÕs 2009 study of charter schools—the most comprehensive ever
done—concluded that 83 percent of them perform either worse or no better
than traditional public schools; a 2010 Vanderbilt University study showed
definitively that merit pay for teachers does not produce higher test scores
for students; a National Research Council report confirmed multiple studies
that show standardized test scores do not measure student learning adequately.
Gates and Broad helped to shape and fund two of the nationÕs most extensive and
aggressive school reform programs—in Chicago and New York City—but
neither has produced credible improvement in student performance after years of
experimentation (Barkan, 2011; Ravitch, 2012a, b).
Neoliberal educational reform, like neoliberal economic
reform, is about advancing capitalist political-economic interests, not
enriching the populace. Neoliberals expunge any challenge to the capitalist
political economy from school curricula. They thus expunge liberal,
communitarian concepts; information dealing with ruling class exploitation and
oppression; and scientific evidence that exposes problems caused by the
capitalist political economy (pollution).
Neoliberal politics engineer all aspects of educational
pedagogy in line with corporate needs. Timed speed tests are useful for socializing
students to respond rapidly to fragmented material in a culture that values
speed and productivity, and immediate return on investment. Timed speed tests
do not encourage relaxed, contemplative, nuanced, creative thinking.
The neoliberal drive to exacerbate, solidify, and
rationalize class divisions generates insight into the neoliberal push for
educational standards – and social standards in general. Standards do not function primarily to assess social value with
the objective of improving sub-standard behavior or tightening accountability
and transparency. (Neoliberals oppose any attempt to increase public oversight
over businesses.) On the contrary, standardized testing, or psychometrics, Òis
best understood as the development of tools for vertical classification and the
production of social valueÓ or class hierarchy (see Garrison,
2009, p. 5; McNamee & Miller,
2009). Educational standardization in contemporary capitalism is not
descriptive, and ameliorative; it is a prescriptive political act that searches
out and exaggerates psychological differences (as statistical tests of
significance find miniscule psychological differences to be ÒsignificantÓ) to
rationalize and exacerbate the class system.
Neoliberal
politicizing of every aspect of education confirms macro cultural psychologyÕs
tenet that cultural and psychological phenomena are political. Neoliberals
properly realize the political function that education has and they properly
seek to align the politics of education with the neoliberal political economy.
Learning is not a neutral, technical apolitical competency. Education is a
cultural activity; it is training ground for cultural participation. There is a
politics of learning.[2]
Neoliberals
have ironically proven the effectiveness of the macro cultural psychological
model of education to transform education. Progressives must appropriate this
model to effectively create a new educational system. Progressives must develop
their own political agenda for education that is rooted in working toward a
progressive political economy and that politicizes every aspect of education
– curriculum, teacher training, pedagogy, school administration,
community participation – to support that system and gain support from it.
That political system and its educational program are subject to discussion;
they are not dogmatic or autocratic (like neoliberalism is).
These
politics must negate the neoliberal features of education and the neoliberal
political economy that underlies them. While classical education and
multicultural education are valuable, to be implemented they must be grounded
in, and informed by, counter-politics (a negative dialectic) that challenge the
status quo. Otherwise, they will be crushed by existing politics of education
and their political economy, as is happening now.
2. Culture
These cases
of education and time illustrate that culture is concrete and political. Culture is not a nebulous Òsharing of
customs,Ó Òcommunicating,Ó Òinteracting,Ó Òsocializing,Ó Òsocial context,Ó or
Òhistorical accumulation of shared practices;Ó nor is culture defined by a
countryÕs name, or by single, abstract variables such as
individualism-collectivism, masculine-feminine, avoidance of uncertainty, long
term orientation, emotional complexity, parental control, or power distance. (Ratner, 2012a, pp. 257-266; 2012b; Ratner & Hui, 2003; Ratner,
2013f). Nor is it spontaneous interpersonal negotiations and agreements
(Ratner 2012a, pp. 248-251). Statements such as Òculture provides templates or
scripts for behavior,Ó or ÒJapanese parents and German parents differ in...Ó call out for specification of what culture is and how
it provides templates for behavior. Macro cultural psychology answers this call
by identifying concrete macro cultural factors of a particular
social system that is rooted in a specific political-economy
controlled by certain political interests that may be autocratic. We investigate
Òthe middle class family in 21st century American consumer-capitalist
society,Ó or ÒConfucian ethical principles,Ó or Òcapitalist sense of time that
was imposed by British colonialism,Ó or Òancien regime sexuality of the
aristocracy,Ó or Òeducational policy of the Ôstate-capitalistÕ government in
contemporary China.Ó
3. Socialization
The
socialization of cultural-psychological phenomena is as much formed by
cultural-political factors/processes as their content is. Colonialism and
corporate neo-liberalism colonize subjectivity to enforce obedience, whereas a
democratic society would foster active psychological development that has an
obvious cultural function in fulfilling democratic practices.
VI. culturally-organized psychological phenomena
are necessary to promulgate a SOCIAL SYSTEM
Psychology
is the subjective element of cultural factors. Psychology must therefore be
developed by cultural factors in order to enact them. Culture needs psychology, it does not eliminate psychology as critics of
cultural psychology believe.
A. EDUCATION
For
instance, education as cultural behavior and cultural factor requires a wide
network of interrelated psychological functions. These include motivation,
reasoning, emotions, memory, self-concept, perception, and imagination. In
particular, education requires an identity of being a learner. One must see
oneself as a learner in order to develop the commitment necessary to engage in
the arduous process of acquiring educational competencies. Coll & Falsafi (2010,
p. 218) express this aptly in saying that Òlearning how to learn requires
learning to be a learner.Ó Identifying with being a learner is the subjectivity
necessary to animate the learning process. ÒThe learner identity consists of
generalized meanings about how one is recognized as a learner by oneself and
others, which then mediate the sense making of the participation and the
perceived sense of recognition as a learner in specific situations and
activities of learningÓ (ibid., p. 220).
It
follows that ÒIf learner identity is to become matter of course for educational
policy making and practice, more research is required based on solid
conceptualizations and with higher levels of concretization. To begin with, we
need a theoretical understanding of its composition, function and development
that permits the analysis of its construction and enables its practical application
as a conceptual technology of the selfÓ (ibid., p.
221).
Macro
cultural psychology explains the subjectivity of the learner as organized by
concrete cultural factors; it is not an abstract, universal ideal, such as
dedication to thoroughly understand profound ideas. We have explained educational
and non-educational (extra-educational) cultural factors that comprise the
identity of a learner. Some of these cultural identities support the identity
of a serious learner while others undermine it. A subverting identity is the
consumer who displays superficial, impulsive, transitory attention to
sensationalistic features of products, or the ÒnerdÓ who is boring and socially
inept. Another distraction from academic learning is the cultural concept of
the learner as possessing innate skills that cannot be substantially improved
by training. An additional distracting learner identity is the concept of
learner as an entrepreneur of knowledge who instrumentally uses education to
pass benchmarks toward a material goal such as a job.
Distracting
identities can only be transformed into serious-learner-identity by altering
the learnerÕs exposure to macro cultural factors, and ultimately by replacing
distracting macro cultural influences (consumerism, entrepreneurialism, low
skilled jobs) with influences that foster serious learning as positive to
individual fulfillment.
B.
GENDER
Gender
identity is another culturally formed psychological phenomenon that is
necessary for successfully participating in and sustaining macro cultural
factors. Macro cultural factors are gendered, i.e., they stand in a gendered
division of labor of male and female roles. Gender identity is part of the
subjectivity necessary to master gendered macro cultural factors and roles. A
discrepant gender identity impedes this mastery.
For
example, math and science are gendered cultural behaviors in that they have
traditionally recruited men, not women. Men, more than women, have been trained
to develop the cognitive and emotional competencies necessary to succeed at
math and science. As such, math and science are identified as masculine, and
masculinity, more than femininity, identifies with math and science. It follows
that masculine gender identity is more comfortable with and attracted to these
activities. Success at math and science is not a purely technical skill; it
requires a subjectivity/psychology that resonates with the skill set. Gender
identity is one key component.
Research
on gender identity in the banking industry supports this analysis. Investment
banking is ÒmasculineÓ in that it is an aggressive, competitive, materialistic industry which has traditionally recruited men. This
masculine identification of the banker role requires masculine gender identity
of bankers. ÒThose who can ÔperformÕ the appropriate forms of masculinity
during this process are the ones who are generally rewarded with a position in
the organizationÓ (North-Samardzic & Taksa, 2011, p. 197). Women who do not
have this orientation will be less successful than men who do.
Bank
managers recognized the importance of gender identity (subjectivity) for participating
in the macro cultural factor of banking, and they actively engineered masculine
gender identity in their male and female employees. ÒImplicit pressure to
conform to and project masculine identity and associated behaviors had an
impact on womenÕs subjective identities...For example, several women from
Markets and Banking noted that they had been Ôperformance managedÕ or ÔtrainedÕ
to be more like their male colleagues, even encouraged to be loud and
assertive.Ó Training women to renounce femininity sustained the male culture in
banking. It created an untenable situation for women. If they do not adopt the
masculine cultural psychology they fail, but if they do adopt it they are
perceived as gender deviants (pp. 208-209).
To
equalize gender participation in math, science, and banking, fundamental
changes in social roles and gender identity are necessary. 1) Change the
gendered division of labor of the cultural activity, as well as its gendered
cultural meaning, so that math and science and banking are represented as
gender neutral or including ÒfeminineÓ competencies. Athletics has been
socially re-engineered in this way to be healthy and fulfilling for women; 2)
Change the cultural organization and meaning of gender identity so that
femininity includes competencies in math and science and banking – as it
now includes sports.
These
complimentary changes in social activity and gender identity will allow women
to participate in male activities as women without having to become gender
deviant. Femininity will enhance their activity, not detract from it.
These
are examples of how enriching a psychological competence – teaching math
and science to women -- can have (indeed, can only occur in concert with)
beneficial social changes in gender and the division of labor of activities. A
recent study confirms that higher math achievement for women depends upon
higher gender equality in social roles such as education, work, and politics.
Importantly, societal gender equality also raises mathematical achievement of
men. National wealth is unrelated to gendered math performance (Kane and Mertz,
2012).
C. CULTURAL IDENTITY
Cultural
templates of identity explain behavior that seems to have no interpersonal
basis and is often attributed to genetic causes. For example, two Chinese twin
girls were adopted as one-year olds by two Canadian couples and grew up 300 km.
apart near Toronto. Although they were raised apart, their behaviors were very
similar. Twin researchers concluded that the similar behavior must have been
due to genetic similarities. However, their cultural identity as twins must be
considered as a factor that motivated them to imitate each other. Their
adoptive parents brought them to visit each other every 6 weeks, and they were
fully aware they were twins. The culturally disseminated, shared cultural
concept (template, script) about twin identity stipulates that twins resemble
each other in action as well as appearance. Adopting this cultural identity
becomes the operating mechanism of behavior; it constitutes powerful motivation
to imitate oneÕs twin despite the absence of continual personal contact or
adult encouragement. Common macro culture compensates for disparate personal
environments.
Cultural
identity explains a wide range of behavior that is not specifically instructed
and rewarded. Identifying with a culture motivates the individual to imitate
cultural models. Identifying with being Muslim, or Chicano, for example, drives
the individual to actively want to become like other Muslims or Chicanos, and search
out and perform culturally appropriate behaviors. Social leaders do not have to
specifically condition every behavior in the individual. Cultural identity does
cultural work and saves the culture effort by utilizing the individualÕs effort
and agency to become complicit in his own cultural socialization. Cultural
identity allows cultural socialization to occur from the side of the individual
– bringing the individual to culture – as well as from the side of
culture – bringing culture to the individual. Cultural identity makes it
easy for citizens to believe the mystifications of their leaders, once they
share a common cultural identification/identity with them. Cultural psychology
plays an important role in maintaining culture.
VII.
Performativity
The
foregoing examples demonstrate that psychology is necessary for maintaining
culture. We turn now to how psychology actually performs cultural factors. J. L. Austin coined the term
Òperformativity.Ó His idea is that speech and action do not merely reflect or
represent a cultural standard, they pragmatically put
it into practice. If social systems are to persist, they must be performed
(reproduced) in the psychology and action of the social members. Performativity
means that individuals are social agents – which makes their
psychology/behavior political acts.
For instance, three-year-olds were shown how to play
a one-player game.
When a
puppet later entered and announced that it too, would play the game, but then
did so in a different way, most of the children objected, sometimes
vociferously. The childrenÕs language when they objected demonstrated clearly
that they were not just expressing their personal displeasure at a deviation.
They made generic normative declarations like, ÒIt doesnÕt work like that,Ó
ÒOne canÕt do that,Ó and so forth. They do not merely disapprove of the puppet
playing the game differently; he is playing it improperly. This behavior is of critical
importance, as it is one thing to follow a norm and it is quite another to
legislate the norm when not involved oneself.
The children had only
to see the adult demonstrate the game – in a straightforward way with no
normative judgments of language – before they jumped to normative
conclusions about how the game should be played (Tomasello, 2010, pp.
37-38).
This is a telling description that demonstrates that
young children actively become social agents who have a sense of shared
intentionality, promulgate social norms as important to obey, and castigate
violations of it. These children did not pursue idiosyncratic, personal
meanings about social behavior, as Òmicro culturalÓ psychologists (such as
Valsiner) claim (Ratner, 2012a chap. 6).
A. PERFORMING
SOCIAL CLASS THROUGH PSYCHOLOGY
Streib
(2011, pp. 349-351) observed 4-year olds at school performing behavior that
reflected their social classes. Her detailed research demonstrates that the
political economy reaches into families where parents initiate rudimentary
interactions with their babies. This equips children with class-structured,
class-appropriate (ÒclassedÓ) behaviors when they arrive at pre-school, where
these are greatly extended by teachers and peers. Streib shows how young children
respond to teachersÕ requirements, and also initiate behavior, based upon
their class preparation; the childrenÕs classed behavior then elicits
reinforcement and extension from the teachers.
Children
at Community Preschool enacted class through their different linguistic styles.
Upper-middle-class preschoolers used their increased willingness to speak,
interrupt, and talk to adults as conversational equals to routinely Òtake the
floor.Ó In doing so, they inadvertently but effectively silenced the working-class
preschoolers, who used fewer words, did not use language to call attention to themselves, and did not talk to adults as they talked to
each other. Similarly, upper-middle-class students used their willingness to
Òtake a standÓ to gain teachersÕ attention, improve their negotiation skills,
and win the bulk of cross-class disputes. Young children do not just know about
class, but are class actors as well (p. 349).
In both Òtaking the floorÓ and
Òtaking a stand,Ó upper-middle-class children seized the bulk of adultsÕ
attention. This attention allowed them to improve their own linguistic skills:
they practiced responding to questions, making public presentations,
debating, rationalizing, and negotiating. They also used their linguistic style
to get their needs met. Seeing social affinity with adult teachers empowered
them to ask for help more often. Their linguistic and social skills also
enabled them to win toys and the turns they were fighting over within the rules
of middle class decorum. As Bourdieu states, language bestows power.
All this made it difficult
for working- class students to take their own stand. The combination of the
childrenÕs class styles made it so that working-class children lost
opportunities to improve their own language skills, lost attention from adults,
lost the ability to get their needs met quickly, and lost cross-class disputes.
This discourages working class 4-year olds from the educational process. They
see the teachersÕ attention drifting to their middle class peers, they see
their own needs going unmet as their peers receive more help, and they learn
that when they argue with an upper-middle-class peer in school it is unlikely
they will win.
Children are politicized agents
of social class; they reproduce it in their social behavior as well as in their
individual reactions.
Streib notes an important consequence of
her research: ÒThis paper calls into question the ability of preschools to
level the playing field. Mixed-class preschools may be sights where inequality
is bolstered instead of dissolved. It should not be assumed that mixed-class
settings are beneficial to working-class studentsÓ.[3]
StriebÕs
children demonstrate how behavior is a cultural means, like an artifact, that
achieves cultural ends.
Of
course, children do not reproduce social class on their own. Their behavior is
encouraged and learned from their social class conditions, including
pedagogical practices. Streib shows how school policy dictates that conflicts
be resolved through verbal negotiations. This policy is viewed as egalitarian
and socially neutral but it is based on middle class behavioral norms and
encourages and rewards middle class behavior. (Conflict resolution policy is
therefore political.) Since upper-middle-class children enter preschool with
the language style that most closely matches the schoolÕs mandated conflict-resolution
style -- using rational arguments to defend a position, and verbal competency
-- they win the majority of the disputes. Conflict resolution policy thus exacerbates
class divisions.
1. Improving Educational Psychology
To
correct these problems within the classroom, teachers should address the class
features of their behavior and studentsÕ behavior. Teachers should make special
effort to help working class students develop competencies that will enable
them to succeed and fulfill themselves in society (Streib, p. 351). The kind of
society that is most fulfilling is open to debate. However, it will surely
include correct grammar, numeracy, abstract reasoning, and using rational
arguments to defend a viewpoint.
In
debating the kinds of competencies that schools should encourage, two errors
must be avoided. 1) Ignoring cultural differences in psychology and attributing
psychological differences to personal or natural competencies of individuals.
This leads to treating lower class students as incapable of handling middle
class material. 2) Idealizing working class kidsÕ educational psychology as
competent and fulfilling. This is the view of cultural relativists who construe
working class educational psychology as a cultural difference but not a
deficiency. Behavioral and cultural reform is deemed unnecessary and even imperialist.
Idealizing
cultural differences under pluralism and multiculturalism ignores the fact that
certain practices are normative and enriching, and that if students do not
acquire them they will be marginalized and incapable of important competencies
such as numeracy, abstract and rational reasoning. Eschewing remediation of sub-standard
children is tantamount to eschewing standards and education in general.
Idealizing
and ignoring cultural differences appear to be contrary, however, both
jeopardize working class children.
B.
AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY
Another
example of psychological phenomena embodying and performing cultural factors is
autobiographical memory, or the manner in which individuals recall and organize
their life experiences in a narrative. Fivush, et al. (2011) argue that
autobiographical memory is actually a social competence for social purposes,
formed by cultural templates that are created and objectified on the macro
cultural level, it is demographically variable, it is a social marker of oneÕs
social position that generated the psychological competency, and it
dialectically promulgates macro cultural factors that form it.
To
begin with, Òautobiography is a critical developmental skill; narrating our
personal past connects us to our selves, our families, our communities, and our
culturesÓ (p. 321). In addition, Òcultures provide organizational and
evaluative frameworks for narrating lives, including canonical cultural
biographies, life scripts, and master narrativesÓ (pp. 322-323). Autobiographical form and content is shaped by a childÕs social
class via her parentsÕ framing of narratives that reflect and reproduce their
class.
Members
of different social classes develop classed autobiographical memories that are
social markers and social competencies (social capital) which
open (or close) social doors. Autobiographical memory thus dialectically
generates class-appropriate behavior, just as classed verbal skills do in the
case of StreibÕs children.
Autobiography
is not primarily an existential quest for meaningful personal experience. It is
a cultural skill that is demanded by various macro cultural factors. This
cultural requirement is what makes autobiography personally important. ÒTo be
accepted as a responsible person, narrators have to demonstrate an
understanding of how their personality and values have developed, influenced
both by lifeÕs pitfalls and happenstance and by their own actions based on
enduring valuesÓ (p. 328). Autobiographical memory is a Òtechnology of self.Ó If
you fail to describe yourself in culturally normative ways, you are not accepted
socially. For example, a major criterion for acceptance into a university is
the manner in which the prospective student tells his life story.
University
admissions committees thus stipulate a cultural concept/template of
autobiography that applicants must incorporate as their own template for how to
narrate their experience. The template includes coherence and expressivity, and
content of how life is to be lived and narrated. Redemption narratives -- where
the individual overcomes negative social pressures -- are preferred (in the
cultural canon) to downward spiral narratives.
Some
prototypical, cultural, master narratives are the John Wayne (JW), Florence
Nightingale (FN), and Vulnerability master narratives. The JW master narrative
refers to the narrator taking a position of courage and stoic resolve during
intensely negative experiences and expressing little or no negative emotion.
Narratives following the FN master narrative express negative emotions as a
result of the traumatic event but these emotions are immediately linked to or
followed by concern for others. Finally, the vulnerability master narrative
allows for the expression of intensely negative emotions and feelings of
helplessness as a result of the negative event (p. 335).
Autobiography
is thus a cultural competency, formed on the macro cultural level in cultural templates, that does cultural work. It
enables individuals to engage in culturally-appropriate
behavior, and play cultural roles. This maintains particular kinds of macro
cultural factors and ensures successful participation in them. Memory, like all
psychological phenomena, is political (see Violi, 2012 for how the
institutionalization/objectification of the display of traumatic historical
events in trauma site museums constitutes a politics of memory that structures
viewersÕ memory, perception, and emotions). Socializing particular forms of
autobiographical memory socializes the individual into the entire culture and
politics that generated those forms and are embodied in them. Psychological
socialization is cultural-political socialization.
The
cultural organization of autobiography encompasses the complex of psychological
skills (language, memory, self-concept) it involves. These are not separate modules, they are interdependent in activities such as autobiographical
memory and thus bear a common cultural-psychological character.
Fivush,
et. al. (p. 327) document
ways that autobiographical competence reflects macro cultural factors mediated
by the family:
In
comparison to Asian mothers overall, American mothers tend to be more
elaborative, asking more open-ended questions, elaborating more on the childÕs
independent contributions, and focusing on the childÕs opinions. Chinese
mothers are more likely to be rated as low-elaborative, asking more yes–no
questions, focusing on factual aspects of the experience, and more rarely
taking account of the childÕs perspective on the event than Euro-American
mothers. Interestingly, 4- to 6-year-old children already portray these
cultural differences in their autobiographical narratives.
Han,
Leichtman, & Wang (1998, p. 702) similarly found Korean
mothers generally did not seek information from children but, instead, prompted
children to confirm information already presented to them. In general, Korean
mothers did not encourage children's introducing their own ideas into the
conversation. In addition, Korean mothers often produced
utterances unrelated to children's previous utterances and expected children to
follow their leads. In contrast, Canadian mothers often followed up and
elaborated on children's utterances, encouraged children to contribute ideas,
and took a partnership rather than a leadership role in conversation.
ChildrenÕs
memory reflected these different socialization processes. Han, et al. (1998) found
that among 4 and 6 year olds, in comparison with Asians, American children
provided more references to specific past events, more descriptives, more
references to internal states, such as evaluations, and more mentions of
themselves relative to others. For example, in response to: Ò"Tell me
every- thing you did at bedtime last night.'' 6-year old girls replied as
follows:
Korean
child: 1 played with my sister. Interviewer: What else did you do? Korean
child: I washed my face and brushed my teeth. Interviewer: What
else? Korean child: Then . . . I watched TV, and after a while, I fell
asleep.
Chinese
child: After I washed my face, I asked
Dad lo tell me a story. Then my mom covered me with the blanket. Then my mom
went over there and read magazines. And finally I fell asleep. Interviewer: What
else did you do?
Chinese
child: Watched TV before going to bed. After watching TV, I went to wash. After
washing, I watched again. Just after I finished dinner, my dad asked me to
practice writing. After practicing writing, he asked me to play the piano. Just
after playing the piano, the TV program began. Then my dad watched TV together
with me. After watching TV, I went to sleep.
U.S.
child: I read, did I read last night? No, I played computer, instead of
reading, but I liked the book better than the computer. Interviewer: What
else did you do? U.S. child: I played with my toys, I think, and then
I'll tell you what game I played on the computer. There's a dictionary thing,
and there's a word finder, and there's hangman, and other games, and three
games, and then there's all different things, and to find a word, you just
click on the word finder, and after two or three letters it just rinds the
word. (pp. 708-709).
In
addition, the earliest memories obtained from a mixed group of Asians and Asian
Americans were, on average, about 6 months later than those of White Americans;
the age of earliest memories of native Koreans and White Americans revealed an
even larger difference of 16.7 months (p. 702).
Cultural
differences in memory reflect cultural needs for different kinds of memory in
order to function in different macro cultural factors. The socializing of
autobiography is also culturally gendered:
Over
time, from age 40 months to age 70 months, both mothers and fathers
increasingly reminisce about more social and emotional experiences with girls
than with boys, and embed emotional experiences in more interpersonally
situated events with daughters but in terms of more autonomous activities with
sons.
By the age of 4 years, children
are already mirroring the gender differences described above in their
independent autobiographical narratives. Girls tell longer personal narratives
than boys, and express more internal state language. By the age of 7, Euro- and
Afro-American females tell more emotional and relationally oriented personal
narratives than boys, whereas boys tell more autonomously oriented narratives
than girls (pp. 327-328).
Gender
differences in memory reflect and reinforce a gendered division of labor. This
cultural-psychological analysis of autobiography reverses the popular idea that
individuals make sense of their experience from their own personal,
psychological perspective, as an existential or metaphysical quest for meaning.
On the contrary, individuals make sense of themselves and events from the
perspective of cultural positions, or roles, in ways that fulfill cultural
roles. The individual does not decompose society into idiosyncratic, personal
fragments; on the contrary, he contributes to cultural coherence by performing
its culturally-shaped, culturally-shared subjectivity.
Ò[Congruence] with the cultural concept of biography, which is shared with
listeners, contributes to creating global coherence in life narrativesÓ (p.
331). This is the way cultures maintain themselves and
produce benefits for individuals.
1. Agency
The
foregoing examples give a new cast to agency. Agency usually denotes individually-created desires and strategies, however, research
reveals that individual agency is political in the sense that it reflects and reproduces
a political system of practices (see Ratner, 1993, 2002, chap. 2, 2009b). Even 4-year
oldsÕ agency is culturally organized to enact class behavior.
North-Samardzic
& Taksa correct the interpersonal, individualistic notion of individuals
Òdoing genderÓ, by emphasizing that organizations Òdo genderÓ: ÒBy
saying the organizations Ôdo genderÕ we mean that organizational cultures
contain speciÞc rules, values, meanings expressed in social situations in which
gender-positioning processes are realized as interpersonal relations in a
public process whereby gender meanings are progressively and dynamically
achieved, transformed and institutionalizedÓ (p. 200). This confirms VygotskyÕs
point that individual behavior is an individualization of social behavior.
VIII.
Epistemology and Methodology
To ascertain the features of psychology that macro cultural
psychology highlights, it is necessary to utilize a particular methodology and
epistemology. Epistemologies and methodologies are bred to discern particular
kinds of phenomena and characteristics; they are not neutral in the sense of
discerning any and all phenomena. Macro cultural psychology must develop a
distinctive epistemology and methodology to discern macro cultural features of
psychology. Two critical elements are objectivism and qualitative methodology.
A.
OBJECTIVISM
Numerous ideologies blind people
to the fact that their psychology/behavior originates in macro cultural
factors, is organized by them, required by them, socialized by them, and
promulgates them. Such
ideologies are functional for obfuscating macro cultural factors that are
oppressive. Indeed, the prevalence of (false) ideologies is a measure of social
oppression (see Callero, 2009, McNamee
& Miller, 2009, Gladwell, 2008 for discussion of the myth of
individualism and meritocracy; religious ideologies similarly explain suffering
in mystical terms such as godÕs will and individual sin which obfuscate
oppressive political macro cultural causes).
Social science methodology must take account of this
cultural-psychological reality. If subjective experience is rendered incapable
of accurately comprehending its origins, characteristics, and function, then social
scientists cannot accept subjective reports as full accounts and understanding
of psychology. Subjective reports (questionnaires, interviews, diaries) must be
supplemented by objective descriptions of macro cultural origins, features, and
functions of subjective experience to discern the extent to which individuals
do and do not understand their behavior/psychology (Ratner, 2011, Ratner, 2013c,
e; Kosik, 1976).
Thomas (2011) used objectivist methodology to identify
lacunae in studentsÕ understanding of racism. Firstly, Ss do not realize that
they practice discrimination, and they see themselves as tolerant of different
ethnicities: ÒThe girls may speak
about stupid racism, but they are as active in its perpetuation through
identity difference and identification as are the violent fighter-boysÓ
(Thomas, 2011, p. 49).
Secondly, Ss do not understand the reasons for their
discriminatory behavior, or other peopleÕs behavior. E.g., the girls attribute
their own racial behavior (that avoids peers of different ethnicity) to simple
personal preference for associating with peers of similar behavior and
appearance (see Ratner, 2011 for examples). The girl students attribute boysÕ
ethnic fighting to intrinsic masculine Òmacho-ism,Ó and do not understand the
social roots of ethnic fighting (Thomas, 2011, p. 71). In these cases, they
focus on the by-products of socio-economic differentiation/exploitation without
perceiving their socio-economic causes. Plato articulated this in his allegory
of the cave where people see shadows without apprehending their causes (see
Kosik, 1976 for a contemporary treatment of this concept). ThomasÕ Ss similarly
feel that sub-classes of people are undesirable and unapproachable without
knowing that their feelings are generated by a deeply-rooted political economic
exploitation of groups of people that makes them appear different and
unfamiliar.
Thirdly, the students do not understand the reasons for
racism in society. And fourthly, they have no viable solution to racism –
beyond Òaccepting everyoneÕs humanityÓ
Thomas uses an objectivist analysis to explain and correct
these four oversights. She identifies discriminatory behavior that Ss generally
do not perceive and certainly do not understand. Secondly, she identifies the reasons
for it in macro cultural factors that institutionalize and tacitly approve
racism (see Alexander, 2010). Thirdly, she explains macro cultural factors that
distort SsÕ self-understanding of what they are doing and why they are doing it.
Fourthly, Thomas articulates macro cultural changes necessary to effectively
overcome racism in society at large and in the SsÕ personal behavior.
Regarding the third and fourth points, Thomas identifies a popular
ideology called Òbanal multiculturalismÓ as one cultural factor that blinds Ss
to understanding the reasons for racism and its presence in their own behavior.
Banal multiculturalism is a superficial view of racism as personal prejudice
about social differences; and it advocates overcoming racism by psychological
means such as being tolerant of differences and appreciating peopleÕs humanity
and individuality. Subjective attitudes of tolerance are construed as having
transcended racism.
This cultural template blinded ThomasÕ Ss to understanding
their own racial behavior. It led them to believe that they have transcended
racism because they subjectively accept culturally diverse individuals. They
therefore interpret their racial behavior as personal preference for similar
peers, not as racism. Banal multiculturalism additionally blinded the Ss from
perceiving that their behavior derives from deep-seated, structural,
political-economic causes of racism – since the ideology construes racism
in terms of personal hostility. The ideological template also prevented Ss from
overcoming racism by challenging its true structural causes.
To understand the basis and character of their behavior, and
to change these, people must reject this distorting cultural ideology, and
replace it with the objectivist analysis that Thomas outlined above and calls Òcritical
multiculturalismÓ (Thomas, 2011, pp. 3-7).
B.
QUALITATIVE METHODOLOGY
Qualitative methodology is central to macro cultural
psychology because it elucidates the full character of psychological phenomena including
their cultural features. Qualitative
methodology utilizes hermeneutics to elucidate psychological and cultural
systems (known as the hermeneutical circle); these systems define, describe,
and explain their elements. Macro cultural psychological methodology uses four
hermeneutic circles or systems: One describes, and explains particular
psychological phenomena in a psychological system. An emotion would be traced
to related perceptions, self-concept, cognitions, motivation, and developmental
processes. In addition, a psychological phenomenon would be situated within a
cultural system, as our examples have illustrated. A third hermeneutic circle
consists of a particular cultural factor situated within a cultural system of
other cultural factors. The fourth hermeneutic circle is to include
psychological phenomena within a cultural system to animate and perform
cultural factors (Ratner, 1997, 2008, 2012a chap.1).
While qualitative methodology is radically different from
positivistic methodology (see Lambdin, 2012 for one point of difference), it
must be rigorour and objective, and not slip into impressionistic reporting of
statements, or draw conclusions that are ungrounded in empirical data.
Models of such rigor are found in grounded theory and
phenomenology. Rigorous procedures include: theoretically justifying oneÕs
hypotheses; clearly defining terms; rigorous coding of narrative data in a way
that is faithful to the words spoken (saturated with the full range of words); grounding
all conclusions in coded responses; obtaining intersubjective agreement about
codes and conclusions among several coders; framing specific questions that focus
on theoretical constructs; framing specific questions about particular macro
cultural factors and their influence on psychology/behavior; using probing
questions to elicit details about cognitive processes, emotions, aspirations,
perceptions, desires, self-concept, and sexuality; using probing questions to
clarify SsÕ responses and compare them to objective assessments of SsÕ behavior
and its cultural context; and encompassing empirical relationships discovered
in research within a theoretical framework.
Thomas uses qualitative methodology to analyze racial
behavior. Unfortunately, she does not systematically employ these rigorous
procedures, and she occasionally slips into ungrounded conclusions. It is
useful to point out these flaws in order to prevent them from recurring.
In one part of her study, Thomas raises the interesting
possibility that sexual desire may have taken on certain racial features in a
racialized society. Sexual desire may be stimulated not only by racial physical
features, but also by racial social behavior such as racial fighting (see
Stoler, 1995). This would be an
interesting example of the acculturation of sexuality and the promulgation of
cultural factors by sexuality.
Unfortunately, on this point, ThomasÕ methodology fails her. She
concludes from her interviews that Òracism articulates
itself as gender and sexual pleasure.Ó ÒThe girls articulate a clear pleasure
with the boysÕ fighting, even as they proclaim their lack of interest.Ó ÒI have
argued that the complexity of their affects and subjectivities point to sexual
desire of racism and racist fightingÓ (ibid., p. 68-69,
72). Yet the brief conversations that Thomas reports do not express these
sentiments.
One conversation went as follows:
Thomas: Do you like it when guys compete for you?
Alexis. No.
Grisselle: Yeah.
Anne: Yeah, like you feel special. But if
they start hurting themselves, no.
Thomas: Then thatÕs not attractive.
Anne: Yeah, because then theyÕll have a bleeding lip and no
eye.
Thomas began this discussion with the theme of liking guys
who compete for girls. She never mentioned or asked Ss to comment on
racial fighting or sexual desire. Nor did any of them mention these. Alexis did
not like the competition. The other two girls liked it but they explicitly
disliked fighting and found it unattractive. Another girl, Sammie said ÒOh god,
I hate that [fighting]; itÕs stupidÓ (p. 69). Yet Thomas insists: ÒThis
exchange is stunning in how clearly it makes the connection between racial
fighting and girlsÕ heterosexual desire.Ó ÒThey desire fightingÓ (pp. 67, 69).
Thomas transforms competition into fighting, fighting into racial fighting
(that was never mentioned), and liking into sexual desire (that was never
mentioned). She strangely transforms her SsÕ explicit rejection of fighting
into their sexual desire for it.
Another conversation (p. 71) replicates these points:
Thomas: So do you think that women like men to be violent?
Speaker 2: Not violent, but they like to be tough.
Thomas: So youÕre attracted to really masculine guys?
Speaker 3: Uh huh.
Speaker 4: Not really
Speaker 5: someone who can take care of me.
Here, speaker 2 explicitly rejects violence. She likes
ÒtoughnessÓ but not violence. Speaker 4 is not even attracted to masculine
guys. Only 3 and 5 are attracted to masculinity. They define
their attraction to masculinity as stemming from its protective function.
They never associate masculinity with fighting, they never mention a desire for
fighting, they never mention a desire for racial fighting, and they never
mention anything sexual about masculinity, protection, or racial fighting.
Thomas abandons objective methodology when she misrepresents what her subjects
say, and claims that girls find racial fighting sexually stimulating.
ThomasÕ departure from rigorous, faithful interpretation of
discourse, gives her license to impose any theoretical construct she favors on
the narrative data. One is heterosexism. She claims that the girlsÕ racism is
promulgated, in part, by their heterosexism, just as it is promulgated by their
racially charged sexual desire. She mentions Òthe heterosexual desire of
racismÓ (pp. 51, 52). She also says Òany words spoken in support of racial
understanding must also be made with the contours of racism, sexism, and
heterosexism in mindÓ (p. 77). She demands that racial understanding include understanding
heterosexism and sexism. But she offers no justification for her requirement.
(Perhaps she believes that all social issues are essentially gender issues and
require a feminist analysis.)
Thomas never defines heterosexism (the dictionary definitions
of which incongruously include assuming that all people are heterosexual,
personally preferring heterosexuality, and discriminating against homosexuals);
she sometimes conflates it with heterosexual desire. She never explains what
the relation between racism and heterosexism might be – how could preferring heterosexuality, or believing all people are
heterosexual, necessarily engender racial fighting? Thomas asks
no specific questions to elicit heterosexism. Thomas never identifies it (in
any of its incongruous forms) in the SsÕ words. Nor does Thomas explain her
insistence that heterosexism contributes to racist fighting and sexual arousal
over it, but never mentions homosexuality in this regard – canÕt
homosexuals be racist, engage in racial fighting, and be sexually stimulated by
it? These problems can be obviated by following the rigorous
procedures of grounded theory and phenomenology enumerated above.
I am not denying that race may pervade sexuality and that
racially charged sexual desire may reciprocally spur racist behavior and racial
violence. Indeed, this would be consistent with macro cultural psychology. I am
saying that Thomas does not prove this with her careless methodology. Improved
qualitative methodology may, or may not, prove it.
IX. Future Directions and
Political Implications of Macro Cultural Psychology
As
macro cultural psychology is in its infancy, the foregoing principles must all
be explored and verified in detail across a wide variety of psychological
phenomena -- including the extent to which psychological phenomena are formed
in macro cultural factors, objectified in them, stimulated, organized, and socialized
by macro cultural factors; the ways in which psychology performs macro cultural
factors, even in intimate, personal activities (Ratner & El-Badwi, 2011).
It is also important to explore the organization and dynamics of culture to
more deeply understand the full origins and characteristics of psychological
phenomena. We must explore the political control of macro cultural factors and
their associated psychology. We must also explore the political function of
psychological phenomena to sustain or challenge the political-economic system. Macro
cultural psychologists will trace debilitating and fulfilling psychological
processes to cultural factors, and use this data to recommend improvements in
cultural factors (Ratner, 2007b, 2009a, 2011, 2012a, b, d; 2013b-f; Ratner
& El-Badwi, 2011). Such recommendations must be concrete negations of
specific problematical features of macro cultural factors. Social change cannot
be effective if it is based on vagaries such as Òmore respect,Ó Òfreedom,Ó Òtransparency,Ó
Òtolerance.Ó[4]
Further
work is needed to examine political assumptions and consequences of
psychological theories, methodologies, and intervention procedures –
i.e., how their politics affect their scientific viability, and how they
sustain or challenge social oppression (e.g., Callero, 2009; McKinnon, 2005).
We must
remain vigilant to develop the distinctive fruitful principles of macro
cultural psychology. We should avoid merging macro cultural psychology with
other approaches in ways that dilute its principles and its integrity. We draw
upon theoretical and empirical contributions from related psychological and
cultural theories that are consistent with macro cultural psychology (Ratner,
1999). These contributions come from social psychology, cultural psychology,
cross-cultural psychology, developmental psychology, anthropology, cultural
geography, history, and sociology. It is fruitless to ÒhybridizeÓ discrepant
viewpoints by inserting hyphens between them and leaving them to eclectically
co-exist (e.g., bio-cultural emotions). Macro cultural psychology approaches
its subject matter in the style of grand theories (paradigms) and OccamÕs law
of parsimony. We identify the shortcomings of other approaches that must be dispelled
if coherent theory building, valid knowledge, and viable social and
psychological improvement are to advance. Refining the logical consistency of macro
cultural psychology involves exploring the extent to which culture transforms
behavior and biological behavioral mechanisms so as to be congruent with cultural
features (and cultural logic).
It is
important to develop research methods – especially qualitative
methodologies -- to investigate the macro cultural origins, characteristics,
and function of psychological phenomena. We need methods to demonstrate how
consumerism affects studentsÕ educational psychology, or how banal
multiculturalism limits studentsÕ understanding of racial behavior. We can learn
from methodologies employed by historians, geographers, anthropologists, and
sociologists that apprehend culture in relation to behavior. We can incorporate
useful procedures, such as experimentation, from positivist methodology, but we
must also be careful to avoid its pitfalls (Lambdin, 2012).
Macro cultural
psychology can develop procedures for intervening in educational and
therapeutic issues in ways that apply insights about cultural origins,
characteristics, and functions of the issues. These
cultural insights will generate fruitful ways to avoid and alter cultural
anchors of problematical issues. Freire called these interventions that raise
consciousness of macro cultural factors ÒconscientizationÓ (Freire, 2000). The term originally
derives from Frantz Fanon's coinage of a French term, conscienciser,
in his 1952 book, Black Skins, White Masks
Hammack (2011, p. 347) explains that the
failure to include conscientization in interpersonal interventions dooms them
to failure: ÒIn their desire for political `neutralityÕ the programs fail to
address issues of power asymmetry that deeply influence the dynamics of the Israeli-Palestinian
conflict. In so doing, they inhibit the extent to which issues of structural
reality can be acknowledged and addressed by youth.Ó
Macro
cultural psychologists need to explore the ways in which macro cultural
psychology and political activism reinforce each other. Macro cultural
psychology explains psychology to social scientists and policy makers, and it
explains cultural politics to psychologists. We should refine and test our
cultural-psychological theoryÕs recommendations for policy reform. We can do
this by comparing our analysis and recommendations to the successes and
failures of others. Validation would come from policy failures by approaches
that eschew macro cultural psychology (negative examples), and from successes
by approaches akin to macro cultural psychology (positive examples). The former
is exemplified in the escalating crises brought about by neoliberal policies (which
I call the politics of disaster); the latter is exemplified by the sound analysis
of the APA Task Force on SES.
References
Alexander, M. (2010). The
new Jim Crow: Mass incarceration in the age of colorblindness. N.Y.: New
Press.
American Psychological
Association (2002). Task force on socioeconomic status.
Washington, D.C.: APA.
Arum, R. & Roksa, J.
(2011).
Academically adrift: Limited learning on college campuses. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Barkan, J. (winter,
2011). Got dough? How billionaires rule our schools. Dissent. http://www.dissentmagazine.org/article/?article=3781
Barry, A. Osborne, T.,
& Rose, N. (1966). Foucault and political reason: Liberalism,
neo-liberalism, and rationalities of government. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.
Burchell, G., Gordon,
C., & Miller, P. (1991). The Foucault effect: Studies in governmentality.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Callero, P. (2009). The
myth of individualism: How social forces shape our lives. London: Rowman
& Littlefield.
Camoletto, R. (2011). Sexual beginners: Accounting for first sexual
intercourse in Italian young people's heterosexual biographies. Sex Education, 3, 1, 315-325.
Coll, C., & Falsafi,
L. (Sept.-Dec., 2010). Learner identity: An educational and analytical tool. Revista
de Educaci—n, 353, 211-233.
Fahs, B. (2011). Performing
sex: The making and unmaking of womenÕs erotic lives. Albany: SUNY Press.
Fivush, R., Habermas, T., Waters, T., Widaad, A. (2011).
The making of autobiographical memory: Intersections of culture, narratives and
identity. International Journal of Psychology, 46, 5,
321-345.
Foster, J. ( Jul.-Aug., 2011). Education and the structural crisis of
capital: The U.S. Case. Monthly Review, 63, 3, 6-37.
Foucault, M. (1997). The subject and power. In M. Foucault, Power (pp.
326-348). N.Y.: New Press.
Freire, Paulo (2007). Pedagogy of the Oppressed. New York: Continuum.
(Originally published 1970).
Friedland, R. (2009),
Institution, practice, and ontology: Toward a religious sociology. Research
in the Sociology of Organizations, 27, 29–68.
Garrison, M. (2009). A
measure of failure: The political origins of standardized testing. Albany:
SUNY Press.
Gladwell, M. (2008). Outliers:
The story of success. N.Y.: Little Brown.
Han,
J., Leichtman, M., & Wang, Q (1998). Autobiographical memory in
Korean, Chinese, and American children. Developmental
Psychology, 34, 701-713.
Jones, P. (2011). The
sociology of architecture: Constructing identities. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.
Kane, J. & Mertz, J. (2012).
Debunking myths about gender and mathematics performance. Notices
of the American Mathematical Society, 59, 10-21.
Kipnis, A. (2011). Governing
educational desire: Culture, politics, and schooling in China. (Chicago:
University Chicago Press.
Kosik, K. (1976). Dialectics
of the concrete: A study on problems of man and world. Dordrecht: Reidel
Publishing Company.
Lambdin, C. (2012).
Significance tests as sorcery: Science is empirical, significance tests are not. Theory
& Psychology, 22, 67-90.
Leonhardt, D. (May 25,
2011). Top colleges, largely for the elite. New York Times, p. B1.
Leontiev, A. N., (1977).
Philosophy in the USSR, Problems of dialectictical
materialism. Moscow: Progress Publishers. (http://www.marxists.org/archive/leontev/works/1977/leon1977.htm)
McKinnon,
S. (2005). Neo-liberal genetics: The myths and moral tales of evolutionary
psychology. Chicago: Prickly Paradigm Press.
McNamee,
S., & Miller, R. (2009). The meritocracy myth.
London: Rowman & Littlefield.
Mansfield, B. (2007).
Privatization: Property and the remaking of nature–society relations. Antipode, 39, 393-405.
Marx, K. (1973). Grundrisse:
Introduction to the critique of political economy. New York: Random House.
(written in1857)
Matsumoto, D.,
& Hwang, H. (2012). Culture and emotion: The integration of biological and
cultural contributions. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, 43, 91-118.
Mauss, M. (1967). The
gift: Forms and functions of exchange in archaic societies. N.Y.: Norton.
(Originally published 1925).
Nanni, G. (2011). Time,
empire and resistance in
settler-colonial
Victoria. Time and Society, 20, 5-33.
Nanni, G. (2012). The colonisation of time. Manchester: Manchester
University Press.
North-Samardzic,
A., & Taksa, L. (2011). The impact of gender culture on womenÕs career
trajectories: An Australian case study. Equality Diversity and Inclusion: An
International Journal, 30, No. 3, 196-216.
Ratner, C. (1989). A social
constructionist critique of naturalistic theories of emotion, J. Mind and
Behavior, 10, 211-230.
Ratner, C. (1991). Vygotsky's sociohistorical psychology and its contemporary
applications. N.Y.: Plenum.
Ratner, C. (1993). Review of D'Andrade & Strauss, Human Motives and Cultural
Models. In Journal of Mind and Behavior, 14, 89-94.
Ratner, C. (1997). Cultural
psychology and qualitative methodology: Theoretical & empirical
considerations. N.Y.: Plenum.
Ratner, C. (1998). Prologue to Vygotsky's
collected works, vol. 5. New York: Plenum.
Ratner, C. (1999). Three approaches to cultural
psychology: A critique. Cultural Dynamics, 11,
7-31.
Ratner, C. (2000). A
cultural-psychological analysis of emotions. Culture
& Psychology, 6, 5-39.
Ratner, C. (2002). Cultural
psychology: Theory and method. N.Y.: Plenum.
Ratner, C., & Hui, L. (2003). Theoretical and methodological problems in cross-cultural
psychology. Journal for the Theory of Social
Behavior, 33, 67-94.
Ratner, C. (2004). VygotskyÕs
conception of child psychology. In R. Rieber (Ed.), The essential
Vygotsky (pp. 401-413). New York: Plenum.
Ratner, C. (2006). Cultural
psychology: A perspective on psychological functioning and social reform.
Mahwah: Erlbaum.
Ratner, C. (2007a). A macro
cultural-psychological theory of emotions. In P. Schultz, & R.
Pekrun (Eds.). Emotions in education (chap. 6).
N.Y.: Academic Press.
Ratner, C. (2007b). The
Cooperative Manifesto: Social Philosophy, Economics, and Psychology for
Co-operative Behavior, Journal of Co-op Studies, 40, 3 (#121), 14-26.
Ratner, C. (2008). Cultural psychology and qualitative methodology:
Scientific and political considerations. Culture and
Psychology, 14, 259-288.
Ratner, C. (2009a).
Cooperativism: A Social, Economic, and Political Alternative to Capitalism. Capitalism, Nature, Socialism, 20, 2, 44-73.
Ratner, C. (2009b).
HarreÕs social philosophy and political philosophy: A social scientific
critique. Journal for The Theory of Social Behaviour, 39,
4, 448-465.
Ratner, C.
(2011). Macro cultural psychology, the psychology of
oppression, and cultural-psychological enrichment. In P. Portes & S.
Salas (Eds.), Vygotsky in 21st Century Society: Advances in cultural
historical theory and praxis with non-dominant communities (chap. 5). NY:
Peter Lang.
Ratner, C., &
El-Badwi, S. (2011). A cultural psychological theory of
mental illness, supported by research in Saudi Arabia. Journal of Social Distress and The Homeless, 20, #3-4,
217-274. (http://www.sonic.net/~cr2/cult psy mental
illness.pdf)
2012a). Macro cultural psychology: A political philosophy of mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
(2012b).
Cultural psychology. In R. Rieber (Ed.), Encyclopedia of the history of psychological theories
(Chapter 28). New York, NY: Springer.
2012c).
Review of Liamputtong, Performing
Qualitative Cross-cultural Research, and Matsumoto and Van de Vijver,
Cross-cultural Research Methods. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 9, 371–374.
2012d). Macro cultural psychology. In J. Valsiner (Ed.), Oxford handbook of culture and psychology (Chapter 10). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
(2013a).
Cooperation, community, and cco-ops in
a global age. New York, NY: Springer.
(2013b). Capitalism and psychology. In T. Teo (Ed.), The encyclopedia of critical psychology. New York, NY: Springer.
(2013c). Emancipation. In T. Teo (Ed.), The encyclopedia of critical psychology. New York, NY: Springer.
(2013d). False consciousness. In T. Teo (Ed.), The encyclopedia of critical psychology. New York, NY: Springer.
Ratner, C. (2013e), Psychology of oppression. In T. Teo (Ed.), The
Encyclopedia of Critical Psychology. N.Y.: Springer.
(2013f). Macro cultural psychology. In T. Teo (Ed.), The encyclopedia of critical psychology. New York, NY: Springer.
Ratner, C. & Esteban Guitart. ( 2011). Historia, conceptos fundacionales y perspectivas
contempor‡neas en psicolog’a cultural [History, foundational concepts, and
contemporary approaches to cultural psychology]. Revista
de Historia de la Psicolog’a.
Ravitch,
D. (Sept. 29, 2011). School ÔreformÕ: A failing grade. New York Review of
Books, 58, 14,
32-35.
Ravitch,
D. (March 8, 2012a). Schools we can envy. The New York
Review of Books.
Ravitch,
D. (March 22, 2012b). How, and how not, to improve the
schools. The New York Review of Books, pp. 17-19.
Russell, B. (1938). Power: A new social analysis. New
York: Routledge.
Simon,
S. (May 25, 2011). Public schools charge kids for basics, frills. Wall Street Journal.
Stearns, P.,
& Stearns. C. (1985). Emotionology: Clarifying the
history of emotions and emotional standards. The American
Historical Review, 90, 813-836.
Stearns,
P. (2011). Consumerism in world history the global
transformation of desire. New York :
Routledge.
Stearns,
P. (2006). American fear : The causes and
consequences of high anxiety. New York :
Routledge.
Stearns,
P. (2005). American behavioral history : An
introduction. New York : New York University
Press.
Stearns,
Peter N. (2003). Anxious parents : A history of
modern childrearing in America. New York : New
York University Press.
Stearns,
Peter. (1999). Battleground of desire : The
struggle for self-control in modern America. New York: New York University
Press.
Stearns,
P. (1994). American cool : Constructing a
twentieth-century emotional style. New York : New
York University Press.
Stearns,
P. (1990). Be a man! : Males in modern society. New York: Holmes &
Meier.
Stoler, A. (1995). Race
and the education of desire: FoucaultÕs ÒHistory of SexualityÓ and the colonial
order. Durham: Duke University Press.
Streib,
J. (2011). Class reproduction by four-year olds. Qualitative
Sociology, 34, 337-352.
Tang,
et al. (July 11, 2006). Arithmetic processing in the brain
shaped by culture. Proceedings of The National
Academy of Sciences, 103, 28, 10775-10780.
Tavernise,
S. (Nov. 16, 2011). Middle class areas shrink as income gap grows, new study
shows. New York Times, p. A16.
Poor dropping further behind rich in school. New York Times (New York edition), p. A1.
(2012, February 10).Thomas,
M. (2011). Multicultural girlhood: Racism, sexuality, and the conflicted
spaces of American education. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
Violi,
P. (2012). Trauma site museums and politics of memory.
Theory, Culture & Society, 29, 36-75.
Vygotsky,
L.S., (1989). Concrete human psychology. Soviet Psychology, 27, 2, 53-77.
Notes
[1] If
this sounds odd, consider the way that personal feelings toward oneÕs children
similarly depend upon the extent to which the youngsters personify cultural
ideals such as academic or athletic prowess. Parents feel proud of their
children when they get good grades or excel in sports, and they feel anxious
and disappointed in their children when they fail to personify cultural ideals.
ParentsÕ feelings towards their children are not purely personal. This social
construction of emotions has an important cultural function of motivating
children to achieve cultural goals to sustain the social order. It is good for
society and for the child. Sexual feelings equally have a cultural basis,
mechanisms, characteristics, and function.
[2] Standards, such as educational criteria,
are windows into the politics of a culture. Struggles over tests, as in contemporary
United States, are really political struggles. There is a politics of testing.
[3] This is an important contradiction to
contact theory which claims that contact among diverse people
sweeps away cultural prejudices and helps everyone express their ÒnaturalÓ
curiosity about and sociality towards others. Acultural notions such as
this are erroneous because macro politics pervade subjectivity and
interpersonal interactions. Rather than relying on a natural sociality,
egalitarian sociality must be constructed at the macro level through structural
change in the ownership and control of institutions and the principles that
govern them.
Hammack (2011,
p. 248) makes this point in his research on mediation between Israeli and
Palestinian youth. One Palestinian, Ali, has participated in two programs
– Seeds of Peace, and Hands of Peace. Yet he remained Òideologically
polarized in his thinking, so determined to `proveÕ his identity as a
Palestinian, and to maintain a fervent anti-israel stance.Ó A 17-year old Palestinian
girl, Leila became more accepting of Israelis after attending a camp Seeds of
Peace with Israeli youth. However, a year later, the political-social-military
conflict betweeb the two countries took its tool on her: ÒI thought about Seeds
of Peace and my experience, and I thought about the situation and the reality
is much harder and much stronger than Seeds of peace. We are seeing things,
seeing facts on TV, but Seeds of peace, itÕs just
words you learned at camp. I really regret the idea that I was in Seeds of
PeaceÉÓ (ibid., p. 318). The bitter national conflict
led Leila to resume her hatred of Jews and to condone suicide bombings which she had repudiated during the Seeds program.
She ultimately said, Ò Jihad is to kill the one who came to take your
landÉÓ (p. 319).
Contact theory did not foster tolerance
and integration because 1) Israelis and Palestinians occupied asymmetrical
status/power, 2) they pursued competitive rather than common goals –
ÒIsraelis and Palestinians are in fierce competition over political and
territorial control and exclusive claims to legitimacy,Ó 3) there was little
cultural and institutional support for mutual understanding (ibid., pp.
249-250). HammackÕs research verifies the macro cultural psychological
principle that macro cultural changes are the basis of interpersonal relations
– and all behavior/psychology. It is futile to attempt to ameliorate
conflict on the interpersonal level through contact while macro conflict
persists. Broad political, economic, social, and military factors must change
in order for interpersonal contact to be accepted and salutary (ibid., p. 345).
HammackÕs
research also demonstrates that identity is not a self-construction, but is
rather structured by macro cultural factors. The macro cultural reality of
national conflict is a central organizing force in the identities of Israelis
and Palestinians (ibid., p. 340; Ratner, 2013f).
Hammack (ibid., pp.254-256) explains that contact theory was conceived by
G. Allport as primarily a bottom-up, individualistic theory of social change
via interpersonal relations. Sheriff tested this theory and found that simple
contact interventions were ineffective. Indeed, contact between competing
groups exacerbated intergroup hostility. Conflict was reduced only when the
experimenters altered the relation between groups by introducing the need for
cooperation to achieve superordinate goals.
The limitations
of contact theory pervade all individualistic interventions. Psychotherapy is
limited because it attempts at treating individuals apart from any concern for
macro cultural factors that are the major causes of psychological dysfunction
(Ratner, 2013e; Ratner & El-Badwi 2011). Attempting to enhance
acculturation of immigrants is similarly limited without structural
opportunities for success.
[4] Under the banner of religious freedom, American jurisprudence allows religious organizations to practice discrimination, and violate the Constitution and Declaration of Independence. A statute known as the Ministerial exception allows religious organizations to discriminate against women in employment and in ordaining leadership positions such as rabbis, Imams, and priests. Religious freedom also allows autocratic religious authorities to prohibit many kinds of sexual and reproductive freedom, including providing birth control by insurance companies that do business with them. Freedom to practice religion masks unfree religious practices. Unprincipled freedom for forces of unfreedom subverts freedom. Consequently, true freedom requires restricting the freedom of religion to impose unfreedom.